91制片厂视频

Special 91制片厂视频

Scientists Find Learning Is Not 鈥楬ard-Wired鈥

By Sarah D. Sparks 鈥 June 04, 2012 11 min read
Ten-year-old Miles Murdough sits in front of brain scans in Dublin, Calif. They show the activity in his brain as he plays the piano.
  • Save to favorites
  • Print
Email Copy URL

Neuroscience exploded into the education conversation more than 20 years ago, in step with the evolution of personal computers and the rise of the Internet, and policymakers hoped medical discoveries could likewise help doctors and teachers understand the 鈥渉ard wiring鈥 of the brain.

That conception of how the brain works, exacerbated by the difficulty in translating research from lab to classroom, spawned a generation of neuro-myths and snake-oil pitches鈥攆rom programs to improve to teaching practices aimed at .

Today, as educational neuroscience has started to find its niche within interdisciplinary 鈥渕ind-brain-education鈥 study, the field鈥檚 most powerful findings show how little about learning is hard-wired, after all.

鈥淲hat we find is people really do change their brain functions in response to experience,鈥 said Kurt W. Fischer, the director of Harvard University鈥檚 Mind, Brain, and 91制片厂视频 Program. 鈥淚t鈥檚 just amazing how flexible the brain is. That plasticity has been a huge surprise to a whole lot of people.鈥

In contrast to the popular conception of the brain as a computer hard-wired with programs that run different types of tasks, said Dr. Jay N. Giedd, a neuroscientist at the National Institute of Mental Health, brain activity has turned out to operate .

Different parts of the brain act like the letters of the alphabet, he said, and by the time a child is 8 months old, the letters are there鈥攖he basic connections have formed in the hippocampus or the prefrontal cortex, say鈥攂ut then through experience, those neural letters activate in patterns to form words, sentences, and paragraphs of thought.

That analogy offers a whole different idea of how the brain develops, both normally and abnormally.

鈥淲hen I first started, we made the mistake of talking about, 鈥極h, the hippocampus is memory; the prefrontal does decisionmaking, impulse control'鈥攁nd it鈥檚 sort of a half-truth,鈥欌 Dr. Giedd said at a recent Learning and the Brain Society talk.

鈥淚 was looking for letters鈥攁 hole in this part of the brain, damage in that part of the brain,鈥 he said. Researchers do find predictable problems, he said, 鈥渂ut it鈥檚 not because of everything that lies in that spot; it鈥檚 because it鈥檚 part of a word or sentence or paragraph that uses that letter a lot. 鈥 The cells that fire together are wired together鈥攁nd grow together.鈥

Moreover, Mr. Fischer and other mind-brain-education researchers said, helping teachers and students understand how the brain changes in response to experience may be the best way to link neuroscience findings to classroom experience.

Rocky Start

91制片厂视频 watchers have had great hopes for dramatic, instruction-changing findings since the early days of educational neuroscience. President George H.W. Bush the 1990s 鈥渢he decade of the brain,鈥 but by the end of it, the promise of the research鈥攎ost of it done with animals鈥攈ad not translated into clear guidelines for instructional practice.

Miles Murdough plays the piano while wired to a brain-scanning device at the Society for Psychology in the Performing Arts in Dublin, Calif. The scans show how his brain reacts to music.

In 1997, the cognitive scientist and philosopher John T. Bruer of the James S. McDonnell Foundation, in St. Louis, declared in a in the American 91制片厂视频al Research Association鈥檚 journal 91制片厂视频al Researcher that directly connecting neuroscience to classroom instruction was 鈥渁 bridge too far.鈥 He urged collaboration among cognitive psychologists, neuroscientists, and educators.

鈥淎ll of our outcome measures, the things we are hoping to see, are not neurological changes; they are behavioral changes,鈥 explained Daniel T. Willingham, a psychology professor at the University of Virginia, in Charlottesville. 鈥淲e don鈥檛 measure how are your dendritic connections, we measure how well you can read.

鈥淭rying to leverage behavioral science [for education] is complicated enough,鈥 he said. 鈥淔or neuroscience to get into the mix, we have . The more distant you get from the level of the classroom, the less likely [the research] is to make a difference in the classroom.鈥

Dr. Kenneth S. Kosik, a neuroscience professor at the University of California, Santa Barbara, and co-director of the Neuroscience Research Institute, helped found the Needham, Mass-based in 1999 to bring together experts from those different fields. But he acknowledged that, 15 years after Mr. Bruer鈥檚 critique, 鈥渨e still have a paucity of real, concrete findings in neuroscience that we can say will change what goes on in the classroom.鈥

Interdisciplinary turf wars are partly to blame for slowing the development of mind-brain-education science, said Dr. Janet N. Zadina, a former high school teacher turned adjunct assistant professor in neurology at Tulane University, in New Orleans, and the winner of the Society for Neuroscience鈥檚 2011 science educator award.

鈥淎t first, it was defensiveness; cognitive psychology wanted to claim it, neuroscience wanted to claim it, educators wanted to claim it, and because the fields have been separate, they were all reinventing the wheel,鈥 she recalled.

Bridging the Disciplines

In the absence of cohesive collaboration among the disciplines, Dr. Zadina said, teachers, policymakers, and education companies were often left to draw their own conclusions from the research, and they often came to overly simplistic or outright wrong conclusions.

One 2011 Arizona State University asked 267 preservice and active teachers to review one of three versions of a fake journal article reporting inaccurate information: One version contained only text, the second contained a graph, and the third had a picture of a brain scan. Teachers were more likely to consider the article containing the brain scan credible, even though it was unrelated to the text.

鈥淭here鈥檚 a reductionism [in which] finding a difference [on a brain scan] is equated with explaining the difference,鈥 said Carol A. Tavris, a psychologist and the author of the 2010 Prentice Hall book Psychobabble and Biobunk: Using Psychological Science to Think Critically About Popular Psychology. 鈥淚t is easy for the public to infer that a snapshot out of context is not a snapshot, but a timeless, unchanging blueprint.鈥

Because most members of the public, including many teachers and researchers, don鈥檛 understand how brain-imaging equipment works, they often develop 鈥渢echnomyopia鈥攖he sense that the technology knows more than I do,鈥 Ms. Tavris said in a keynote address to the Association for Psychological Science last month.

Yet technology used in brain imaging, including functional magnetic-resonance imaging, or fMRI, and magnetoencephalography, can be thrown off by movement, she said, and readings are easily misinterpreted.

Toolbox

Researchers use a variety of new technologies to take measurements on the brain, including:

鈥 Electroencephalography (EEG) measures changes in electrical voltage in neurons by using sensors placed on the scalp. The technology is helpful in measuring rapid changes in the brain in response to stimuli, and is often used to diagnose epilepsy. It can be particularly useful for research involving children, because it does not require the child to remain as still as other measures do. However, EEG readings are most accurate when measuring neuron activation close to the skull, and may be less helpful for examining activity deeper within the brain, in areas such as the hippocampus.

鈥 Magnetoencephalography (MEG), uses arrays of highly sensitive magnetometers to measure the electrical currents produced by brain activity. It is often used to map the areas of the brain associated with different cognitive tasks, or to identify electrical signatures associated with conditions such as autism. It is sensitive to movement, however, and there is only one meg in the country, at the Institute for Learning and Brain Sciences at the University of Washington in Seattle, that is sized for research on infants and toddlers.

鈥 Functional magnetic-resonance imaging (FMRI) measures changes in blood flow during brain activity. It can measure changes in all areas of the brain with a clearer pinpointing of location than through EEG or MEG, but it requires longer brain activity. It is often paired with EEG or meg to provide a more coherent map of activity. The technology can be difficult to use with children because it requires a subject to lie still in a closed tube.

SOURCE: Sarah D. Sparks

鈥淣euroscientists love their brain scans,鈥 Dr. Kosik agreed. 鈥淭hey can be interpreted in all kinds of ways, as though they鈥檙e Tarot cards, when you are talking to a teacher, 鈥 then you go home and say, 鈥楾hat was a beautiful picture with lots of brain stuff, but, OK, what does it mean, what do I do next?鈥 鈥

For example, early brain-imaging studies on dyslexia pointed to differences between dyslexic and typical readers in the back left of the brain鈥攁 region associated with sound processing. That led some educators to consider dyslexia a hard-wired physical problem in the brain and therefore harder to treat with educational interventions. Yet emerging research on language development shows that a person processes both letter sounds and the direct meanings of words, and uses different neural connections to comprehend a language like English, in which the same letters can have multiple sounds, than for Finnish, which has more stable phonemes, the sounds that make up spoken words. Later studies have shown various subtypes of dyslexia respond differently to interventions, and in some cases, those with the disorder can have an edge in types of pattern recognition like the kind astronomers use.

鈥淲e got very excited about that鈥 finding, Mr. Fischer said, 鈥渂ecause it shows we need to stop thinking about simple disabilities; we need to think about patterns of understanding, patterns of processing. Different kids learn differently.鈥

Ms. Tavris told 91制片厂视频 Week she sees little potential in the near future for neuroscience to do more than reinforce findings from psychology and behavioral sciences.

Yet Mr. Fischer argued that methods for measuring brain growth and activity have all been developed in the past 10 to 15 years, and 鈥渋t takes a while when you have a new tool to figure out how to use it effectively.鈥

Moreover, he added, laboratory-based psychological studies can be just as difficult to translate into classroom practice.

鈥淚n cognitive science,鈥 he said, 鈥測ou flash a word on the screen for a 10th of a second鈥攖hat鈥檚 not what happens in a classroom.鈥

Dr. Kosik, Mr. Fischer, and others in mind-brain-education research agree that the neuroscience evidence in their field has been sketchy so far, but they argue that criticizing the field for replicating educational findings from psychology misses the point of mind, brain, and education.

鈥淲hat we鈥檙e trying to do is cover the discipline [of learning] more broadly,鈥 Mr. Fischer said. 鈥淲e need to figure out how to do more practical research. We need to have research in school settings and learning environments to become a norm.鈥

鈥淲e do it in medicine for just about everything,鈥 he said. 鈥淲e need to do it in education.鈥

Mind-brain-education experts have also called for the creation of more laboratory schools, similar to teaching hospitals in medicine, in which teachers can test the implications of emerging research. Doing so could take translations of mind-brain-education research beyond 鈥渏ust a talk on the brain,鈥 Dr. Zadina said, to 鈥渁n overhaul of practice, the basis of what we do.鈥

Broad changes in perspective will be more important to shifting teacher practice than fMRI results of a particular intervention, according to Marc Schwartz, the director of the Southwest Center for Mind, Brain, and 91制片厂视频 at the University of Texas at Arlington. 鈥淚 tell [teachers] right from the beginning that silver bullets don鈥檛 exist,鈥 Mr. Schwartz said, but argued that doesn鈥檛 mean neuroscience findings, such as those on brain plasticity, can鈥檛 be relevant to education.

鈥淰ariability is often overlooked as a gift rather than a nemesis; teachers think, 鈥榯hese students are so different, they can鈥檛 adapt to what I鈥檓 teaching,鈥 鈥 he said. 鈥淢ind-brain-education [study] has given us a more flexible view of children, and to the extent teachers accept that, they become more powerful teachers.鈥

The 300-student in Oshkosh, Wis., does not look promising from the outside: A hulking, windowless concrete throwback to the 1970s open-concept school design, it has virtually no interior classroom walls.

Yet for the past six years, the charter school鈥攚ith a 2011-12 enrollment in which 45 percent of students are living in poverty and 30 percent require special education services鈥攈as been building just the sort of teaching environment that could help translate brain research into classroom practice throughout the district.

Learning Laboratory

The 50-odd teachers are on monthly listservs for new research and discuss the findings during regular lunch discussions. 鈥淚f it鈥檚 been studied, we鈥檒l probably talk about it: neuroscience, cognitive psychology, behaviorism,鈥 Principal B. Lynn Brown said.

鈥淎 student鈥檚 brain physically changes every day, and the way we teach either enhances or impairs it,鈥 she said, noting that she and her teachers hold summer seminars for other district staff on the brain鈥檚 flexibility and response to instruction. 鈥淲e have to ask ourselves as educators, what does our practice say about what we believe? We are explicitly teaching thinking skills.鈥

As a laboratory school, the Jacob Shapiro school regularly provides training seminars for teachers and administrators from other schools and districts and plays host to observing researchers; the children have learned to ignore visitors moving in and out of the classrooms.

During one such visit last fall, the 2nd grade science class was learning about butterflies. The students took a lot of breaks, all of them active; students started out with wild wriggles and moved to smaller and slower movements, quieting. Students with difficulty regulating their emotions learn about executive function in the prefrontal cortex of the brain, and how, like lifting weights to build a muscle, they can practice self-control and attention, according to Kristine Hutchinson, the school鈥檚 music teacher, who also helps implement Jacob Shapiro鈥檚 discipline program.

Students who became stressed or frustrated during class could seek one of the school鈥檚 many 鈥渟afe spots,鈥 quiet nooks with soft seating and pictures reminding them of those lessons in calming themselves down, such as taking deep breaths, squeezing a compression ball, or writing out their anxieties. 鈥淚t鈥檚 not a time-out spot; it鈥檚 a place kids voluntarily go,鈥 said Ms. Hutchinson. 鈥淲e check in with them, but we鈥檙e teaching them to self-regulate.鈥

Three times a week, students also take a 30-minute class in metacognitive skills, learning about how their brains work and how to think about their own learning and problem-solving.

In one session, thinking-skills teacher Shirley Rose set out a spatial-logic problem for the 3rd graders to teach directions. She drew out the image of a man standing in the center of a group of objects, and the children talked through ways to figure out which direction the man was facing and which objects he could see at any one time.

鈥淲hen I first started this, sometimes you are driven to just finish this sheet and make sure all the answers are correct, and really that鈥檚 not the point,鈥 said Ms. Rose. 鈥淭he point is to get them to understand their own thinking and strategies.鈥

Even young children can understand the basic concepts of brain plasticity鈥攆or example, that their brains are malleable and will change as they practice something鈥攁nd she said 鈥渋t becomes a very powerful thing for them, that just because they have an error, it鈥檚 not this terrible, dreadful thing.鈥

Coverage of 鈥渄eeper learning鈥 that will prepare students with the skills and knowledge needed to succeed in a rapidly changing world is supported in part by a grant from the William and Flora Hewlett Foundation, at .
A version of this article appeared in the June 06, 2012 edition of 91制片厂视频 Week as Neuroscientists Find Learning Is Not 鈥楬ard-Wired鈥

Events

Recruitment & Retention Webinar Keep Talented Teachers and Improve Student Outcomes
Keep talented teachers and unlock student success with strategic planning based on insights from Apple 91制片厂视频 and educational leaders.鈥
This content is provided by our sponsor. It is not written by and does not necessarily reflect the views of 91制片厂视频 Week's editorial staff.
Sponsor
Families & the Community Webinar
Family Engagement: The Foundation for a Strong School Year
Learn how family engagement promotes student success with insights from National PTA, AASA鈥痑nd leading districts and schools.鈥
This content is provided by our sponsor. It is not written by and does not necessarily reflect the views of 91制片厂视频 Week's editorial staff.
Sponsor
Special 91制片厂视频 Webinar
How Early Adopters of Remote Therapy are Improving IEPs
Learn how schools are using remote therapy to improve IEP compliance & scalability while delivering outcomes comparable to onsite providers.
Content provided by 

EdWeek Top School Jobs

Teacher Jobs
Search over ten thousand teaching jobs nationwide 鈥 elementary, middle, high school and more.
Principal Jobs
Find hundreds of jobs for principals, assistant principals, and other school leadership roles.
Administrator Jobs
Over a thousand district-level jobs: superintendents, directors, more.
Support Staff Jobs
Search thousands of jobs, from paraprofessionals to counselors and more.

Read Next

Special 91制片厂视频 How Much Does Special 91制片厂视频 Truly Cost? Finally, an Answer Is on the Horizon
A new federally funded research study will provide the first national assessment in 25 years of spending on students with disabilities.
10 min read
Budget analysis calculator, data, budget sheet.
iStock/Getty
Special 91制片厂视频 Sudden Fame for Tim Walz's Son Brings Attention to Non-Verbal Learning Disorder
Searches on Google spiked for the the teen鈥檚 name and non-verbal learning disorder.
4 min read
Democratic vice presidential nominee Minnesota Gov. Tim Walz hugs his son Gus during the Democratic National Convention Wednesday, Aug. 21, 2024, in Chicago.
Democratic vice presidential nominee Minnesota Gov. Tim Walz hugs his son Gus during the Democratic National Convention Wednesday, Aug. 21, 2024, in Chicago.
Erin Hooley/AP
Special 91制片厂视频 Opinion This Is My Reality as a Student With Dyslexia
A high schooler reflects on what teachers should know to make school better for students with learning differences.
Maxwell J. Lesny
4 min read
A man in a maze, concept idea art of lost solution and problem, surreal mystery painting, conceptual artwork, 3d illustration
Jorm Sangsorn/iStock + 91制片厂视频 Week
Special 91制片厂视频 AI's Potential in Special 91制片厂视频: What Teachers and Parents Think
A report examines parents' and educators' perspectives on AI use for students with disabilities.
3 min read
 Illustration of artificial intelligence and motherboard in the shape of a lightbulb with two head profiles on either side, both containing circuit imagery inside and a female sitting on top of one working on a laptop.
DigitalVision Vector